A Reaction-Diffusion Model Generating Turing Patterns

From random walks to reaction-diffusion

In the previous lesson, we introduced the model of a particle randomly walking through a medium. But what exactly do random walks have to do with Alan Turing and zebras?

Turing’s insight was that remarkable high-level patterns could arise if we combine particle diffusion with chemical reactions, in which colliding particles interact with each other. Such a model is called a reaction-diffusion system, and the emergent patterns are called Turing patterns in Turing’s honor.

We will consider a reaction-diffusion system having two types of particles, A and B. The system is not explicitly a predator-prey relationship, but you may like to think of the A particles as prey and the B particles as predators for reasons that will become clear soon.

Both types of particles diffuse randomly through the plane, but the A particles diffuse more quickly than the B particles. In the simulation that follows, we will assume that A particles diffuse twice as quickly as B particles. In terms of our random walk model, this faster rate of diffusion means that in a single “step”, an A particle moves twice as far as a B particle.

STOP: Say that we release a collection of A and B particles at the same location. If the particles move via random walks, and the A particles diffuse twice as fast as the B particles, then on average, how much farther from the origin will the A particles be than the B particles after n steps?

We now will add three reactions to our system. First, A particles are added into the system at some constant feed rate f. As a result of the feed reaction, the concentration of the A particles increases by a constant number in each time step.

Note: We will work with a two-dimensional simulation, but in a three-dimensional system, the units of f would be in mol/L/s, which means that every second, there are f moles of particles added to the system for every liter of volume. (Recall from your chemistry class long ago that one mole is 6.02214076 · 1023 particles, called Avogadro’s number.)

Second, B particles are removed from the system at a constant kill rate k. As a result of the kill reaction, the number of B particles in the system will decrease by a factor of k in a given time step. That is, the more B particles that are present, the more B particles will be removed.

Third, our reaction-diffusion system includes the following reaction involving both particle types. The particles on the left side of this reaction are called reactants and the particles on the right side are called products.

A + 2B → 3B

To simulate this reaction on a particle level, if an A particle and two B particles collide with each other, then the A particle has some fixed probability of being replaced by a third B particle, which could vary based on the presence of a catalyst and the orientation of the particles. This probability directly relates to the rate at which this reaction occurs. This third reaction is why we compared A to prey and B to predators, since we may like to conceptualize the reaction as two B particles consuming an A particle and producing an offspring B particle.

The three reactions defining our system are summarized by the figure below.

Before picture of reaction-diffusion system After picture of reaction-diffusion system
A visualization of our reaction-diffusion system. (Left) The system contains both types of particles and two collisions. The two A particles shown with dashed lines are not yet present. (Right) Two of the A particles are fed into the system, two of the B particles die out, and a B particle replaces an A particle after the collision of two B particles and an A particle.

Before continuing, we call your attention to a slight difference between the feed and kill reactions. In the former, the concentration of A particles increases by a constant in each time step. In the latter, the concentration of B particles decreases by a constant factor multiplied by the current concentration of B particles. If we were using calculus to model this system, then letting [A] and [B] denote the concentrations of the two particle types, we would represent the feed and kill reactions by the following two differential equations:

d[A]/dt = f

and

d[B]/dt = -k · [B].

Parameters are omnipresent in biological modeling

A parameter is a variable quantity used as input to a model. Parameters are inevitable in biological modeling (and data science in general), and as we will see, changing parameters can cause major changes in the behavior of a system.

Four parameters are relevant to our reaction-diffusion system: the feed rate (f) of A particles, the kill rate (k) of the B particles, the predator-prey reaction rate (r), and the diffusion rate (i.e., speed) of the the B particles. We do not need to set the diffusion rate of the A particles, since we know that their diffusion is twice that of the B particles.

We think of all these parameters as dials that we can turn, observing how the system changes as a result. For example, if we raise the diffusion rate, then the particles will be moving around and colliding with each other more, which means that we will see more of the reaction A + 2B → 3B.

STOP: What will happen as we increase or decrease f or k?

In the following tutorial, we will initiate our reaction-diffusion system with a uniform concentration of A particles spread across the grid and a tightly packed collection of B particles in the center of the grid. When we return from this tutorial, we will see if any high-level patterns form.

Visit tutorial

Changing reaction-diffusion parameters produces different emergent Turing patterns

For many parameter values, our reaction-diffusion system is not very interesting. The following animation is produced when using parameter rates in CellBlender of f = 1000 and k = 500,000. It shows that if the kill rate is too high, then the B particles (colored red) will die out more quickly than they can be replenished by the reaction with A particles (colored green), and so only A particles will remain.

On the other hand, if f is too high, then the increased feed rate will cause an increase in the concentration of A particles. However, the increased concentration of A particles will also cause more interactions between A particles and pairs of B particles, causing the concentration of B particles to explode. The following simulation has the parameters f = 1,000,000 and k = 100,000.

The interesting behavior in this system lies in a sweet spot of the f and k parameters. For example, consider the following visualization when f is equal to 100,000 and k is equal to 200,000. We see a clear stripe of predators expanding outward against a background of prey, with subsequent stripes appearing at locations where a critical mass of predators can interact with each other.

When we hold k fixed and increase f to 140,000, the higher feed rate increases the likelihood of B particles encountering A particles, and so we see even more stripes of B particles.

As f approaches k, the stripe structure becomes chaotic and breaks down because there are now many clusters of B particles colliding and mix with each other. The following animation shows the result of raising f to 175,000.

Once f is equal to k, the stripes disappear, as shown in the video below. We might expect this to mean that the A and B particles will be uniformly mixed. Instead, we see that after an initial outward explosion of B particles, the system displays a mottled background, with pockets having higher or lower concentration of B. Pay attention to the following video at a point late in the animation. Although the concentrations of the particles are still changing, there is much less large-scale change than in earlier videos. If we freeze the video, our eye cannot help but see patterns of red and green clusters that resemble mottling.

The Turing patterns that emerged from our particle simulations are a testament to the human eye’s ability to find organization within the net behavior of tens of thousands of particles. For example, take another look at the video we produced that showed mottling in our particle simulator. Patterns in this simulation are noisy — even in the dark red regions we will have quite a few green particles, and vice-versa. The rapid inference of large-scale patterns from small-scale visual phenomena is one of the tasks that our brains have evolved to perform well.

You may still be skeptical, since the patterns in the above videos do not have the concrete boundaries that we might expect of animal stripes and spots. Yet when we closely examine an animal with skin patterns, we can see that, like a pointillist painting, the patterns we infer on a higher level are just the net result of varied individual points. The figure below shows an example of this effect for zebrafish skin.

image-center Zooming in on the striped skin of a zebrafish shows that each stripe is formed of many differently colored cells, and that the boundaries of the stripes are more variable than they may seem at lower resolution. Image courtesy: JenniferOwen, Wikimedia Commons (adapted by Kit Yates).

Turing’s patterns and Klüver’s form constants

The particle simulations in the previous subsection may evoke the adjective “trippy”. This is no accident.

Research dating to the 1920s has studied the patterns that we see during visual hallucinations, which Heinrich Klüver named form constants after studying patients who had taken mescaline.1 Form constants, such as cobwebs, tunnels, and spirals, occur across many individuals, regardless of the cause of their hallucinations.

image-center A few of Heinrich Klüver’s form constants. Image courtesy: Lisa Diez, Wikimedia Commons.

Over five decades after Klüver’s work, researchers would determine that form constants having different shapes originate from simpler linear stripes of cellular activation patterns in the retina. The retina is circular, but the brain needs to convert this cellular image into a rectangular field of view; as a result, when the linear patterns are passed to the visual cortex, the hallucinating brain contorts them into the spirals and whirls that we see.2 Some researchers3 believe that the patterns in the retina caused by hallucinations are in fact Turing patterns and can be explained by a reaction-diffusion model in which one type of neuron acts as a predator and another acts as prey.

Streamlining our simulations

Despite using advanced modeling software that has undergone years of development and optimization, each of the simulations in this lesson took several hours to render because they require us to track the movement of tens of thousands of particles over thousands of generations.

We wonder if it is possible to build a model of Turing patterns that does not require so much computational overhead. In other words, is there a simplifying speed-up that we can make to our model that will still produce Turing patterns? We will turn our attention to this question in the next lesson.

Next lesson

  1. H. Klüver. Mescal and Mechanisms of Hallucinations. University of Chicago Press, 1966. 

  2. G.B. Ermentrout and J.D. Cowan. “A Mathematical Theory of Visual Hallucination Patterns”. Biol. Cybernetics 34, 137-150 (1979). 

  3. J. Ouellette. “A Math Theory for Why People Hallucinate”. Quanta Magazine, July 30, 2018. https://www.quantamagazine.org/a-math-theory-for-why-people-hallucinate-20180730/ 

Comments